Which of the following subsistence activities is an example of a delayed return system?

Read Online (Free) relies on page scans, which are not currently available to screen readers. To access this article, please contact JSTOR User Support . We'll provide a PDF copy for your screen reader.

With a personal account, you can read up to 100 articles each month for free.

Get Started

Already have an account? Log in

Monthly Plan

  • Access everything in the JPASS collection
  • Read the full-text of every article
  • Download up to 10 article PDFs to save and keep
$19.50/month

Yearly Plan

  • Access everything in the JPASS collection
  • Read the full-text of every article
  • Download up to 120 article PDFs to save and keep
$199/year

Log in through your institution

Purchase a PDF

Purchase this article for $20.00 USD.

How does it work?

  1. Select the purchase option.
  2. Check out using a credit card or bank account with PayPal.
  3. Read your article online and download the PDF from your email or your account.

journal article

The Significance of Storage in Hunting Societies

Man

New Series, Vol. 18, No. 3 (Sep., 1983)

, pp. 553-571 (19 pages)

Published By: Royal Anthropological Institute of Great Britain and Ireland

https://doi.org/10.2307/2801597

https://www.jstor.org/stable/2801597

Read and download

Log in through your school or library

Alternate access options

For independent researchers

Read Online

Read 100 articles/month free

Subscribe to JPASS

Unlimited reading + 10 downloads

Purchase article

$20.00 - Download now and later

Abstract

It is commonly supposed that hunters neither invest their labour in the expectation of a delayed return nor store their food to any significant extent. Though true of some, these suppositions are certainly not true of all. This article focuses on storage as one manifestation of delayed return in hunting societies. Three senses of storage are distinguished: ecological, practical and social. Ecological storage, an interruption in the flow of nutrients from animals and plants to human consumers, is related to waste, the deflection of this flow. The practical activity of setting aside harvested produce is seen as a response to the temporal scheduling of resource extraction, transport, preparation and consumption. Social storage, by contrast, refers to the convergence of rights to specific resource upon a single interest, and is an aspect of the rationality of resource conservation. The practical storage involved in time budgeting does not necessarily entail social storage, which limplies the perception of scarcity of resources regarded as property or wealth. The deferment of access to resources conceived in this social sense cannot be deduced from delays in the practical processes of production. Appropriative labour is specific to agricultural and pastoral production, as opposed to purely extractive hunting and gathering; therefore 'delayed returns' have a social significance in the former which is lacking in the latter. Yet in peoples' conceptual categories this opposition may be confounded, yielding perhaps what amounts to an ideological transformation from hunting to farming.

Publisher Information

The Royal Anthropological Institute of Great Britain and Ireland (RAI) is the world's longest-established scholarly association dedicated to the furtherance of anthropology (the study of humankind) in its broadest and most inclusive sense. The Institute is a non-profit-making registered charity and is entirely independent, with a Director and a small staff accountable to the Council, which in turn is elected annually from the Fellowship. It has a Royal Patron in the person of HRH The Duke of Gloucester KG, GCVO.

  • Loading metrics

Open Access

Peer-reviewed

Research Article

  • Kenneth B. Vernon ,
  • Peter M. Yaworsky ,
  • Brian F. Codding

Subsistence strategy mediates ecological drivers of human violence

  • Weston C. McCool, 
  • Kenneth B. Vernon, 
  • Peter M. Yaworsky, 
  • Brian F. Codding

Which of the following subsistence activities is an example of a delayed return system?

x

  • Published: May 23, 2022
  • https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0268257

Figures

Abstract

Inter-personal violence (whether intra- or inter-group) is a pervasive yet highly variable human behavior. Evolutionary anthropologists suggest that the abundance and distribution of resources play an important role in influencing differences in rates of violence, with implications for how resource conditions structure adaptive payoffs. Here, we assess whether differences in large-scale ecological conditions explain variability in levels of inter-personal human violence. Model results reveal a significant relationship between resource conditions and violence that is mediated by subsistence economy. Specifically, we find that interpersonal violence is highest: (1) among foragers and mixed forager/farmers (horticulturalists) in productive, homogeneous environments, and (2) among agriculturalists in unproductive, heterogeneous environments. We argue that the trend reversal between foragers and agriculturalists represents differing competitive pathways to enhanced reproductive success. These alternative pathways may be driven by features of subsistence (i.e., surplus, storage, mobility, privatization), in which foragers use violence to directly acquire fitness-linked social payoffs (i.e., status, mating opportunities, alliances), and agriculturalists use violence to acquire material resources that can be transformed into social payoffs. We suggest that as societies transition from immediate return economies (e.g., foragers) to delayed return economies (e.g., agriculturalists) material resources become an increasingly important adaptive payoff for inter-personal, especially inter-group, violence.

Citation: McCool WC, Vernon KB, Yaworsky PM, Codding BF (2022) Subsistence strategy mediates ecological drivers of human violence. PLoS ONE 17(5): e0268257. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0268257

Editor: Raven Garvey, University of Michigan, UNITED STATES

Received: September 30, 2021; Accepted: April 23, 2022; Published: May 23, 2022

Copyright: © 2022 McCool et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Data Availability: All of the data presented in this paper can be found in the Supporting Information.

Funding: The authors received no specific funding for this work.

Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Introduction

Inter-personal violence, whether intra- or inter-group, is a persistent attribute of human societies, though the degree of violence varies [1]. Explanations for coalitional violence include a wide variety of cultural [2–5] and evolutionary [6–17] hypotheses. These long-debated explanations center on the ultimate causes of collective violence while evolutionary approaches focus on the adaptive payoffs for individual participation. Recent research also explores the co-evolution of inter-group violence with group cooperation, altruism, and the emergence of complex social systems [16, 18, 19]. Those that propose inter-group violence to be a rare or maladaptive behavior [20–22] have been effectively countered in many cases [23–26], although important debate persists [7].

Here we examine how ecological and economic factors influence the adaptive payoffs for inter-personal violence and in doing so explain cross-cultural variation. We focus on the proposed adaptive payoffs for participation in inter-group violence–although these payoffs should impact intra-group violence as well–which are most often posited to be (a) social rewards such as status, mating opportunities, and alliance formation [8, 11, 13, 27], or (b) the procurement and defense of scarce resources [6, 28–30]. Our central question is whether macroscale variation in the abundance and distribution of local resources has a structuring effect on the adaptive payoffs for violence, and whether subsistence economy mediates this relationship. It is critical then that we introduce how social and material payoffs relate to violent interactions.

Many scholars hypothesize that inter-group violence serves as a resource procurement strategy and, thus, propose that the benefits of violence are high when individuals compete for high-ranking resources that are scarce and distributed in patches that can be effectively privatized [6, 28–30]. In the most general sense, inter-group violence is proposed to be a subsistence activity to procure or defend valued resources. Typically, these strategies target group-level rewards, such as territory, that elicit collective action problems [11, 31, 32]. As such, participation in resource violence must be accompanied by social rewards, such as status [11], or a disproportionate share of the loot that can then be mobilized to enhance fitness. Scarce, patchily distributed resources, thus, provide the initial incentive for violence, with social rewards or the accumulation of personal wealth providing the impetus for individual participation.

Other researchers propose that participation in inter-group violence is best explained as a strategy to directly gain, co-opt, or defend status, alliances, or mating opportunities (i.e., social rewards), with resource procurement and defense playing an irrelevant or epiphenomenal role [11, 13, 27, 33–35]. This may arise when socioeconomic conditions (e.g., mobility, resource surplus, privatization) preclude the use of violence to amass and own resources. In this sense, participation in violence is motivated by direct social rewards and should be more frequent when the abundance of local resources allows for energetic surpluses to be allocated into violent competition [30, 32, 36–38]. In this case, rich environments more frequently “finance” costly inter-group violence.

Relating these alternative explanations to resource conditions (Fig 1), we expect that (1) if inter-group violence is incentivized by the procurement of material resources and attendant fitness payoffs, then rates of violence should be highest in marginal, heterogeneous environments with scarce, clustered resources that can be monopolized [6, 28–30]. Alternatively, (2) if inter-group violence is primarily a strategy to directly acquire social rewards, with resource acquisition being an epiphenomenal motivation [11, 33, 34], then rates of violence should be highest in environments where abundant and predictable resources provide energy surpluses that can be diverted into violent inter-group (and likely intra-group) competition [30, 32, 36–38]. A third, somewhat less obvious possibility is that subsistence economy plays a key mediating role, whereby the transition to delayed-return economies (e.g., agriculturalists) allows for the accumulation of wealth, generating greater incentives for resource violence.

Fig 1. Conceptual model.

Expectations for the two primary explanations. Punnett squares display the expected payoffs for violence as a function of environmental productivity (resource abundance) and heterogeneity (resource dispersion).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0268257.g001

We evaluate the importance of resource conditions relative to levels of violence using a global archaeological dataset of delayed-return agricultural societies and a global ethnographic dataset of immediate-return forager and mixed-economy horticultural societies (Fig 2). We combine archaeological and ethnographic datasets because (a) archaeological violence datasets are extremely limited for foragers and horticulturalists, while ethnographic research lacks data on violence among complex, agricultural societies–particularly non-industrial and non-western populations prior to the 20th century, and (b) it is vital that we assess whether socioeconomic features mediate the ecological drivers of violence, as discussed above. We combine these datasets to test how levels of violence are affected by the spatial distribution of net primary productivity (NPP), both its mean (resource abundance) and standard deviation (resource dispersion). The novel dataset presented here expands on previous evaluations of coupled archaeological and ethnographic data [e.g., 18] and allows us to quantitatively explore the question of whether levels of violence differ by ecological conditions and modes of subsistence.

Materials and methods

Following Bowles [18], we developed a database of proportional violence (hereafter referred to as “PV”) using archaeological and ethnographic data and categorized all sampled groups into three modes of subsistence (MoS): foraging, horticulture, and farming (see, S1 File). We define MoS simply as benchmarks along the continuum from immediate-return to delayed-return economies along with the related features of subsistence and social organization such as mobility, surplus, storage, and privatization [39, 40]. Pastoralists are not included in this study due to the lack of sufficient observations to run a separate empirical model. In all, the database contains 53 populations from seven World regions (Fig 2 and S1 and S2 Files).

While archaeological and ethnographic measures of violence are not identical, they have been used to good effect in several prominent studies [18, 41]. The dataset developed here builds off prior studies by providing additional archaeological and ethnographic samples and improves precision by excluding archaeological groups with small sample sizes.

Archaeological measure of violence

The archaeological database derives from several dozen peer-reviewed publications that report proportions of violent skeletal trauma. Bioarchaeologists have robust methods for distinguishing intentional violent trauma from accidents and post-mortem damage [42–44]. This database operationalizes PV by defining it as the number of individual skeletons with evidence of violent trauma divided by the total skeletal sample for each archaeological observation (i.e., region or society). Published data that reports ante-mortem (healed) and peri-mortem (non-healed) skeletal trauma were included if they (1) reported proportion of violent trauma that exclude accidents and post-mortem damage, (2) contained sample sizes of approximately n = 100 individuals or greater (with the exception that small islands contain smaller samples sizes), and (3) contained site or region-specific location data. In all, 34 samples from seven World regions were included from 25 sources (S1 and S2 Files). Since most sources do not report peri-mortem trauma rates, we rely on generalized inter-personal violence. Because this type of data does not distinguish intra- vs inter-group violence, we treat the database as a long-term record of generalized inter-personal violence for a given area, which is standard practice for cross-regional bioarchaeological studies [e.g., 6, 42, 45–48]. However, as intra-group lethal and non-lethal violence tend to be low in human societies [1, 2], we expect that changes in population-level inter-personal violence will largely reflect fluctuations in inter-group violence. Furthermore, the payoffs for violence outlined in this paper (e.g., mating, status, and resource competition) should influence general inter-personal violence, regardless of whether it is intra- or inter-group. While certain global regions are not included due to lack of data with adequate sample sizes, the current samples in the archaeological database span a variety of net primary productivity (NPP) values (Fig 3), which allows for robust hypothesis testing. To provide a more representative sample of archaeological data, research will be needed to produce and disseminate high-resolution trauma datasets from a wider variety of economic and ecological contexts.

Fig 3. A density plot for the observations (points) in our dataset.

Color represents the density of observations (societies) along both environmental dimensions (mean NPP and standard deviation in NPP).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0268257.g003

Ethnographic measure of violence

Our ethnographic database derives from peer-review publications and online scholarly sources. This database operationalizes PV by defining it as the number of observed or reported acts of lethal violence divided by the estimate of the total ethnographic population size. Proportions of lethal violence are obtained first through aggregate sources (e.g., ourworldindata.org) and then corroborated using eight original published sources (SS2 File). In all cases, proportions of violent death are obtained from the most recent publications. The ethnographic database contains 19 populations from seven World regions (Fig 2).

Data compatibility

Because the archaeological database contains generalized inter-personal trauma and the ethnographic database contains violent deaths, these two databases are not perfectly compatible. Perhaps most importantly, we cannot compare these two datasets to evaluate differences in absolute rates of violence. We must acknowledge that the ethnographic rates would be higher if they included all skeletal trauma, and the archaeological rates would be lower if they included only lethal trauma. That said, both data types produce similar types of measurements: incidents of inter-personal violence divided by population estimates. In addition, lethal and sub-lethal violence tend to co-occur, as does intra- and inter-group violence [49–52] and should offer comparable measures of relative rates of violence in each society, thus informing us about the functional relationship between that relativized rate and the environment.

What does this mean for our models? Primarily, it means that they cannot provide an unbiased estimate of the intercept, understood as the marginal or baseline rate of violence. However, they should offer unbiased estimates of the coefficients, where those capture functional responses to environmental conditions. Fortunately, for this analysis, the intercepts are largely irrelevant, for all we need to evaluate our hypotheses are the model coefficients. That is, we are more concerned with relative rather than absolute rates of violence.

Environmental productivity

As a proxy for environmental productivity, we relied on terrestrial net primary productivity (NPP), which approximates photosynthesis, measuring the amount of energy that is turned into mass and thereby approximating the amount of new growth biomass available to consumers. The rasters containing mean and SD NPP were compiled between 2000 to 2015 at a 1-km resolution using remotely sensed data from the MODIS instrumentation on NASA’s Terra satellite, processed and provided by the Numerical Terradynamics Simulation Group at the University of Montana [53, 54]. These were converted to kg/C/m2 by multiplying by the scaling factor 0.0001 [55]. We then extract NPP estimates for a 50km buffer generated around a geospatial centroid for each of the societies in our sample and map out the densities of the observations over space (Fig 3).

Modern NPP has been used to predict numerous prehistoric phenomena including population density [56–58], habitat colonization [59], resource scarcity [6] and more. While NPP provides a measure of modern productivity, the relative NPP ranking of each region should have remained consistent over time due to their broad geographic range representing general physiographic regions rather than variants within single ecosystems [60, 61]. To better illustrate this point, we take as an example two regions included in our sample: the North American Colorado Plateau (CP) and the Illinois river valley (IRV). While the absolute NPP of these two regions certainly fluctuated throughout the centuries, there was no point during which the “mountain grassland and scrublands” of the CP were more environmentally productive (higher NPP) than the “tropical and subtropical dry broadleaf forests” of the IRV [60, 61]. The relative differences in environmental productivity between these two regions are due to their differing physiographic characteristics resulting in a marginal ecology with the former and a productive ecology with the latter, which have remained comparatively consistent for millennia. The same can be said about the relative variation in NPP between the other regions compared in our analysis and the observations clustered therein (e.g., the lowland neotropics of the Amazon Basin versus the Canadian boreal forests). At most, changes in absolute NPP would only serve to exaggerate or flatten the modeled curves but would not lead to trend reversals.

Analytical methods

We first evaluate our sample for potential spatial autocorrelation in proportion of violence (sigma) using Monte Carlo simulations of a Moran’s I Test as implemented by the moran.mc function in the R spdep package [62] (SS2 File). This includes an analysis across all MoS and within each MoS. We find the former to be significant (MI = 0.37, p = 0.03) but primarily driven by autocorrelation among farming populations (MI = 0.39, p = 0.045), as there was no autocorrelation among foragers (p = 0.663) or horticulturalists (p = 0.295). To test whether this may indicate a latent spatial process an additional Moran’s I Test on the residuals of the full model (see below) revealed no significant spatial autocorrelation (MI = 0.169, p = 0.165), suggesting that model coefficients captured any latent spatial process in the response.

To test our research questions, we fit three generalized linear models (GLMs) with a binomial distribution and log link appropriate to proportional data using quasi-likelihood estimation to account for overdispersion in our response variable (PV). We generate a null model without predictors, a base model with only our environmental predictor variables (mean and SD NPP), and a complete explanatory model with environmental predictors and MoS, specified as an interaction term to test for differences in intercepts and slopes, with farming as the reference class. We then perform a likelihood ratio test to evaluate whether each increase in model complexity provides sufficient gain in explanatory power. For each model, we report PV as a function of mean and SD NPP along with coefficient estimates and standard errors shown as log of the odds ratio. All analyses are conducted in the R programming environment and language [63] (For more details about our analysis, please see SS2 File).

Results

The base empirical model that includes all forager, farmer, and horticulturalist populations results in a significant model improvement from the null model (X2 = 0.47, p = 0.0458, Table 1) and shows that violence positively covaries with mean NPP (β = 0.513, p = 0.0475), and has a limited response to NPP SD (β = -1.355, p = 0.1771), suggesting that the overall proportion of violence is slightly higher in productive environments (r2 = 0.08), as predicted by the social rewards hypothesis (Table 1).

Table 1. Results of binomial GLMs evaluating proportional violence (PV) as a function of mean and SD NPP.

Coefficient estimates and standard errors are shown as log of the odds ratio. Coefficients in the full model are relative to the farming reference class.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0268257.t001

The more complex empirical model accounting for MoS (forager, horticulture, farmer) results in a significant model improvement from the base model (X2 = 2.13, p < 0.0001, r2 = 0.34, Fig 4). The proportion of violence as a function of environmental productivity significantly differs between foragers and farmers (p = 0.0002), between farmers and horticulturalists (p < 0.0001), and between foragers and horticulturalists (p = 0.0001) (Table 1 and Fig 4). The proportion of violence as a function of environmental heterogeneity also differs significantly between these MoS. For foragers and horticulturalists, violence positively co-varies with environmental productivity and homogeneity, while for farmers violence negatively co-vary with productivity and homogeneity.

Fig 4. Model results showing the predicted response of proportional violence (PV) for each subsistence mode to every combination of mean environmental productivity (NPP) and the standard deviation in environmental productivity (NPP SD).

Predicted values are constrained to their observed range in the data. Marginal response plots for each MoS are available in S1 File.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0268257.g004

To summarize, including MoS as an interaction term significantly improves model fit, showing that proportions of generalized violence among foragers and farmers in particular exhibit opposite covariance with environmental productivity and heterogeneity.

Discussion

The empirical model shows that farmers have higher proportions of violence in low productivity, high heterogeneity environments and the lowest proportions of violence in high productivity, homogeneous environments. The opposite trend is present among foragers (and to a lesser extent, horticulturalists), with the highest proportions of violence occurring in high productivity, homogeneous environments and the lowest proportions of violence occurring in low productivity, heterogeneous environments. These results show that in order to explain ecologically driven variation in human violence it is necessary to account for different subsistence economies. The opposite covariance along subsistence lines warrants explanation in relation to the adaptive motivations for violence.

These results support the expectation that the adaptive payoffs for inter-group (and inter-personal) violence are increasingly mediated by resource availability as societies develop delayed-return economies reliant on resource surpluses, storage, and privatization, all of which enable the accumulation and distribution of wealth that can be mobilized to enhance or maintain fitness rewards such as status, marriage opportunities, and alliances [64–66]. For immediate-return economies, like mobile foraging, the benefits of violence are high when resources are abundant, homogeneous, and predictable, as their economic and social organization renders resource procurement an epiphenomenal payoff relative to direct social rewards. The result of this transition is a fundamental change in the socioecological conditions that promote collective violence. While our explanation is tentative and will require further testing, it provides a single evolutionary rationalization for these seemingly divergent findings. We further unpack this explanation below.

Violence over social and material rewards

Violence used to gain, co-opt, or defend direct mating and marriage opportunities has long been a topic of controversy and empirical debate [e.g., 67]. Ethnographers report evidence showing adult males frequently fight over mating opportunities and real or perceived infidelities [13, 33, 67]. Others find that violent conflict can result in the disproportionate accumulation of wealth that can be used to gain direct mating opportunities via social systems such as bride price [12]. In either case, those who hypothesize direct mating opportunities as a prime motivation for violence emphasize its potential to directly elevate inclusive fitness [8, 12, 13, 33]. However, explanations that promote mating opportunities without reference to status competition are problematic, as reputation is tied up in all competitive activities, including mating contests.

Afterall, competition takes numerous forms, including behavioral strategies aimed at signaling fitness quality to potential mates, allies, and competitors [68–71]. Many scholars provide evidence that participation in coalitional violence acts as a form of costly signaling intended to deter competitors, encourage alliance formation, and attract mates [8, 11, 28, 35]. Observers of these costly displays benefit from honest signals that are useful for conflict avoidance, mate choice, and competitor evaluation. As the costs of participating in coalitional violence tend to be high [6, 72], dishonest signals should be rare and easily detectable. These payoffs have clear fitness consequences, as status consistently links to reproductive success [73]. Violence as a costly display also includes fights over direct mating opportunities, which signal one’s willingness to defend or co-opt access to reproductive partners. To summarize, we argue that the available evidence points to status competition as a key adaptive payoff for human violence, with conflict over mating opportunities being a component of larger reputational contests. Environmental conditions then must be linked to the variable payoffs for violent status contests, and how they are mediated by subsistence economy.

For foragers and horticulturalists, organization or participation in collective violence can produce or signal embodied and relational wealth [74] that may translate into reproductive success [73]. For agriculturalists, organization or participation in collective violence can produce material wealth that can enhance fitness [65, 73] as well as be passed down inter-generational channels [65, 74]. The key distinction is that foragers receive high payoffs for direct behavioral competition while farmers (and other delayed-return societies) benefit from resource-based competition. Seeming exceptions prove the rule: in prehistoric California, delayed-return hunter-gatherers who rely on the storage of privatized resources follow the violence pattern observed here among agriculturalists [6].

The positive covariance between violence and environmental productivity among foragers and horticulturalists speaks to the capacity of rich environments to finance frequent costly displays and render other forms of signaling less effective. For example, when high-ranking prey items are abundant and predictable, they may require less knowledge and skill to capture relative to environments where similar game is scarce and difficult to acquire [75]. In this case, the same strategy (e.g., big game hunting) will produce larger signaling payoffs in the poor environment relative to the rich one. Such ecological constraints on the number of honest signals that can be conveyed may result in a greater emphasis on status-driven violence, where abundant resources permit greater energy being allocated into violent competition. More frequent displays in productive environments may intensify competition, with positive feedback resulting in higher rates of violence. The negative covariance between violence and standard deviation in environmental productivity suggests that homogeneous environments promote violence either because abundant resources are spread evenly across the landscape, permitting a greater number of individuals to pay the steep costs of participation, or because other venues for status competition are rendered less effective when resources are evenly distributed.

Contrasting an immediate-return economy such as mobile foraging to a delayed-return economy like farming, the latter provides greater opportunities for resources to be stored, owned, and accumulated as wealth, which can be used to facilitate marriages or enhance status via conspicuous displays or generosity [12]. Following marginal utility theory, resource-related violence should be especially intense when resources are scarce, and stored in large, dense, monopolizable packages [6, 28–30]. This is because the utility of a resource diminishes with the amount of that resource an individual possesses [76] and risk preferences vary as a function of relative wealth [77, 78]. As a result, farmers (and likely pastoralists) with abundant resources should be more tolerant of theft and more risk-averse (in this case, violence-avoiding), whereas those with scarce resources should be less tolerant of theft and more risk-prone (violence-prone) [28]. To summarize, among delayed-return economies low productivity, high variance environments should promote violence from those seeking resources that can be translated to fitness payoffs and those incentivized to defend them [6, 12, 29].

Violence and starvation

One might argue that the link between resource scarce environments and violence may indicate starvation-induced behavior. Afterall, if cooperative solutions to acute starvation are unavailable, individuals will use zero-sum strategies (including violence) to obtain necessary subsistence resources regardless of MoS or other intervening variables such as age or sex. Nonetheless, we expect starvation-induced violence to be rare for three reasons.

First, if survivorship is frequently in jeopardy, one should see far broader demographic participation in resource conflict. Afterall, if starvation negatively impacts everyone, resource-based violence should involve a wide demographic subset of the population and far greater sex-based parity. Instead, intergroup violence most often involves the organization and participation of young adult males [28, 79, 80]. This is not to say that exceptions do not exist, but rather to highlight the general pattern.

Second, many of those who attempt to empirically tie violence to acute starvation or chronic nutrient deprivation have failed to do so effectively [e.g., 81], with violence more consistently resulting in increased food shortages [11, 34].

Third, as stated above, the ‘resource procurement’ hypothesis implies the capture or defense of group-level rewards, such as clustered resources or territory, that elicit collective action problems [25, 32]. Recent studies have suggested that collective action problems can be overcome when participation in inter-group violence serves to accumulate resource wealth that can be translated into fitness rewards, such as marriage opportunities [12] or status [11], suggesting resource procurement may be a proximate means of gaining social rewards rather than alleviating resource shortages.

This is not to say that human populations never undergo starvation, or that violence and starvation are totally unrelated. Rather, these factors indicate that starvation induced violence cannot explain the full, or even normal, range of variability in violence, and is thus not the primary target of selection. Instead, we argue that resource-based violence is primarily a strategy for obtaining material resources that can increase fitness by enhancing status and facilitating marriages and alliances.

Conclusion

We find that violence-promoting environmental conditions are opposite for foragers and farmers, with violence among foragers peaking in rich, homogeneous environments, and violence among agriculturalists peaking in marginal, heterogeneous environments. We argue that the opposite covariance reflects the increasing role of material resources in motivating violent competition over fitness-related payoffs such as status, marriage opportunities, and alliances. These results yield insight into the behavioral ecology of violent behavior by assessing how variability in ecological conditions and subsistence economies explain cross-cultural differences in rates of violence.

The results of this paper should, of course, be treated as presenting tentative and testable hypotheses for future investigation. More robust tests of the expectations laid out in this paper will require a larger pool of archaeological trauma datasets that differentiate between ante-mortem and peri-mortem trauma, especially among mobile hunter-gatherers, horticulturalists, and pastoralists. Ethnographic datasets must be broadened to evaluate rates of violence among complex agricultural societies (particularly non-western populations).

The present study represents a preliminary effort to address the behavioral ecology of violence across the ecological and socioeconomic spectrum. Future theoretically-grounded research will benefit from a focus on explaining environmentally mediated variability in violent behavior, with an emphasis on individual motivations as a function of differential payoffs. The evolutionary perspective presented here can help to address questions regarding the frequency and intensity of human conflict, thus providing a foundation for developing science-based tools that may aid in conflict mitigation both in the present and the future.

Supporting information

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the University of Utah Archaeological Center, the PLoS ONE editors, and the anonymous reviewers.

References

  1. 1. Wrangham RW, Michael LW, Martin NM. Comparative rates of violence in chimpanzees and humans. Primates. 2006;47(1):14–26. pmid:16132168
    • View Article
    • PubMed/NCBI
    • Google Scholar
  2. 2. Boehm C. Hierarchy in the forest: the evolution of egalitarian behavior. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA; 1999.
    • 3. Ember CR, Ember M. Resource unpredictability, mistrust, and war: A cross-cultural study. Journal of Conflict Resolution. 1992;36(2):242–262.
      • View Article
      • Google Scholar
    • 4. Ferguson BR, Whitehead LN. War in the tribal zone. School of American Research Press, Santa Fe, NM; 1992.
      • 5. Otterbein KF. Internal war: A cross‐cultural study 1. American Anthropologist. 1968;70(2):277–289.
        • View Article
        • Google Scholar
      • 6. Allen MW, Bettinger RL, Codding BF, Jones TJ, Schwitalla AW. Resource scarcity drives lethal aggression among prehistoric hunter-gatherers in central California. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2016;113(43):12120–12125. pmid:27790997
        • View Article
        • PubMed/NCBI
        • Google Scholar
      • 7. Beckerman S, Erickson PI, Yost J, Regalado J, Jaramillo L, Sparks C, et al. Life histories, blood revenge, and reproductive success among the Waorani of Ecuador. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2009;106(20):8134–8139.
        • View Article
        • Google Scholar
      • 8. Chagnon NA. Life histories, blood revenge, and warfare in a tribal population. Science. 1988;239(4843):985–992. pmid:17815700
        • View Article
        • PubMed/NCBI
        • Google Scholar
      • 9. Durham WH. Resource competition and human aggression, part I: A review of primitive war. The Quarterly Review of Biology. 1976;51(3):385–415.
        • View Article
        • Google Scholar
      • 10. Field JS. Explaining fortifications in Indo‐Pacific prehistory. Archaeology in Oceania. 2008;43(1):1–10.
        • View Article
        • Google Scholar
      • 11. Glowacki L, Wrangham RW. The role of rewards in motivating participation in simple warfare. Human Nature. 2013;24(4):444–460. pmid:24008817
        • View Article
        • PubMed/NCBI
        • Google Scholar
      • 12. Glowacki L, Wrangham RW. Warfare and reproductive success in a tribal population. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2015;112(2):348–353.
        • View Article
        • Google Scholar
      • 13. Macfarlan SJ, Erickson PI, Yost J, Regalado J, Jaramillo L, Beckerman S. Bands of brothers and in-laws: Waorani warfare, marriage and alliance formation. Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 2018;285(1890):20181859. pmid:30381383
        • View Article
        • PubMed/NCBI
        • Google Scholar
      • 14. Tooby J, Cosmides L. The evolution of war and its cognitive foundations. Institute for evolutionary studies technical report. 1988;88(1):1–15.
        • View Article
        • Google Scholar
      • 15. Wrangham RW. Evolution of coalitionary killing. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 1999;110(S29):1–30. pmid:10601982
        • View Article
        • PubMed/NCBI
        • Google Scholar
      • 16. Zefferman MR, Mathew S. An evolutionary theory of large‐scale human warfare: Group‐structured cultural selection. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews. 2015;24(2):50–61. pmid:25914359
        • View Article
        • PubMed/NCBI
        • Google Scholar
      • 17. Zefferman MR, Baldini R, Mathew S. Solving the puzzle of human warfare requires an explanation of battle raids and cultural institutions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2015;112(20):E2557–E2557. pmid:25911638
        • View Article
        • PubMed/NCBI
        • Google Scholar
      • 18. Bowles S. Did warfare among ancestral hunter-gatherers affect the evolution of human social behaviors? Science. 2009;324(5932):1293–1298. pmid:19498163
        • View Article
        • PubMed/NCBI
        • Google Scholar
      • 19. Rusch H. The two sides of warfare. Human Nature 2014;25(3):359–377. pmid:24928285
        • View Article
        • PubMed/NCBI
        • Google Scholar
      • 20. Ferguson RB. Ecological consequences of Amazonian warfare. Ethnology. 1989;28(3):249–264.
        • View Article
        • Google Scholar
      • 21. Fry DP. (Ed.). War, peace, and human nature: the convergence of evolutionary and cultural views. Oxford University Press; 2012.
        • 22. Moore JH. The reproductive success of Cheyenne war chiefs: A contrary case to Chagnon’s Yanomamo. Current Anthropology. 1990;31(3):322–330.
          • View Article
          • Google Scholar
        • 23. Allen MW, Jones TL. (Eds.). Violence and warfare among hunter-gatherers. Left Coast Press; 2014.
          • 24. Chagnon N. Response to Ferguson. American Ethnologist. 1989;16(3):565–570.
            • View Article
            • Google Scholar
          • 25. Glowacki L, Wilson ML, Wrangham RW. The evolutionary anthropology of war. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization. 2017;178:963–982.
            • View Article
            • Google Scholar
          • 26. Hames R. Pacifying hunter-gatherers. Human Nature. 2019;30(2):155–175. pmid:30953274
            • View Article
            • PubMed/NCBI
            • Google Scholar
          • 27. Macfarlan SJ, Walker RS, Flinn MV. Chagnon NA. Lethal coalitionary aggression and long-term alliance formation among Yanomamö men. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2014;111(47):16662–16669. pmid:25349394
            • View Article
            • PubMed/NCBI
            • Google Scholar
          • 28. Daly M. Killing the competition: Economic inequality and homicide. Routledge; 2017.
            • 29. Dyson‐Hudson R, Smith EA. Human territoriality: an ecological reassessment. American Anthropologist. 1978;80(1):21–4.
              • View Article
              • Google Scholar
            • 30. Georgiev AV, Klimczuk AC, Traficonte DM, Maestripieri D. When violence pays: a cost-benefit analysis of aggressive behavior in animals and humans. Evolutionary psychology. 2013;11(3):147470491301100313. pmid:23864299
              • View Article
              • PubMed/NCBI
              • Google Scholar
            • 31. Glowacki L, von Rueden C. Leadership solves collective action problems in small-scale societies. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2015;370(1683):20150010. pmid:26503683
              • View Article
              • PubMed/NCBI
              • Google Scholar
            • 32. Wiessner P. Collective action for war and for peace: A case study among the Enga of Papua New Guinea. Current Anthropology. 2019;60(2):224–244.
              • View Article
              • Google Scholar
            • 33. Chagnon NA. The Yanomamo. Cengage Learning; 2012.
              • View Article
              • Google Scholar
            • 34. Kelly RC. The evolution of lethal intergroup violence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2005;102(43):15294–15298. pmid:16129826
              • View Article
              • PubMed/NCBI
              • Google Scholar
            • 35. Patton JQ. Thoughtful warriors: Status, warriorship, and alliance in the Ecuadorian Amazon [dissertation]. University of California, Santa Barbara; 1996.
              • 36. Georgiev AV. Energetic costs of reproductive effort in male chimpanzees [dissertation]. Harvard University; 2012.
                • 37. Golabek KA, Ridley AR, Radford AN. Food availability affects strength of seasonal territorial behaviour in a cooperatively breeding bird. Animal Behaviour. 2012;83(3):613–619.
                  • View Article
                  • Google Scholar
                • 38. Southwick CH. An experimental study of intragroup agonistic behavior in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Behaviour. 1967;28(1/2):182–209.
                  • View Article
                  • Google Scholar
                • 39. Smith CS. Hunter-gatherer immediate-return systems: A case study from the mid-Holocene Elk Head site, Big Horn Basin, Wyoming. Plains Anthropologist. 2020;65(256):298–324.
                  • View Article
                  • Google Scholar
                • 40. Woodburn J. Hunter-Gatherers Today and Reconstruction of the Past. In: Gellner E, editor. Soviet and Western Anthropology. Columbia University Press, New York; 1980. p. 95–117. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0161-6420(80)35094-x pmid:7243217
                  • 41. Gómez JM, Verdú M, González-Megías A, Méndez M. The phylogenetic roots of human lethal violence. Nature. 2016;538(7624):233–237. pmid:27680701
                    • View Article
                    • PubMed/NCBI
                    • Google Scholar
                  • 42. Arkush E, Tung TA. Patterns of war in the Andes from the Archaic to the Late Horizon: insights from settlement patterns and cranial trauma. Journal of Archaeological Research. 2013;21(4):307–369.
                    • View Article
                    • Google Scholar
                  • 43. Lovell NC. Trauma analysis in paleopathology. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 1997;104(S25):139–170.
                    • View Article
                    • Google Scholar
                  • 44. Ortner D. Differential diagnosis of skeletal injuries. In: Kimmerle EH, Baraybar JP, editors. Skeletal trauma: Identification of injuries resulting from human rights abuse and armed conflict. CRC Press; 2008. p. 21–86.
                    • 45. Kohler TA, Ortman SG, Grundtisch KE, Fitzpatrick CM, Cole SM. The better angels of their nature: Declining violence through time among prehispanic farmers of the Pueblo Southwest. American Antiquity. 2014;79(3):444–464.
                      • View Article
                      • Google Scholar
                    • 46. Lambert PM. Patterns of violence in prehistoric hunter-gatherer societies of coastal southern California. In: Frayer DW, Martin DL, editors. Troubled times: Violence and warfare in the past. Gordon and Breach Publishers; 1997. p. 77–109.
                      • 47. Milner GR, Anderson E, Smith VG. Warfare in late prehistoric west-central Illinois. American Antiquity. 1991;56(4):581–603.
                        • View Article
                        • Google Scholar
                      • 48. Steadman DW. Warfare related trauma at Orendorf, a middle Mississippian site in west‐central Illinois. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 2008;136(1):51–64. pmid:18186503
                        • View Article
                        • PubMed/NCBI
                        • Google Scholar
                      • 49. Lutz C. Militarization. In: Nugent D, Vincent J, editors. A companion to the anthropology of politics. Blackwell Publishing; 2007. p. 318–331.
                        • 50. McCool WC, Tung TA, Coltrain JB, Accinelli Obando AJ, Kennett DJ. The character of conflict: A bioarchaeological study of violence in the Nasca highlands of Peru during the Late Intermediate Period (950–1450 CE). American journal of physical anthropology. 2020;174(4):614–630. pmid:33382102
                          • View Article
                          • PubMed/NCBI
                          • Google Scholar
                        • 51. Nordstrom C. Deadly myths of aggression. Aggressive Behavior. 1998;24(2):147–159.
                          • View Article
                          • Google Scholar
                        • 52. Tung TA. Making warriors, making war: Violence and militarism in the Wari Empire. In: Scherer AK, Verano J, editors. Embattled bodies, embattled places: War in pre-Columbian America. Harvard University Press; 2014. p. 227–256.
                          • 53. Zhao M, Heinsch FA, Nemani RR, Running SW. Improvements of the MODIS terrestrial gross and net primary production global data set. Remote sensing of Environment. 2005;95(2):164–176.
                            • View Article
                            • Google Scholar
                          • 54. Zhao M, Running SW. Drought-induced reduction in global terrestrial net primary production from 2000 through 2009. Science. 2010;329(5994):940–943. pmid:20724633
                            • View Article
                            • PubMed/NCBI
                            • Google Scholar
                          • 55. Running SW, Zhao M. Daily GPP and annual NPP (MOD17A2H/A3H) and year-end gap-filled (MOD17A2HGF/A3HGF) products NASA Earth Observing System MODIS land algorithm. (MOD17 user’s guide version 4.2, 2019).
                            • 56. Bradshaw CJ, Ulm S, Williams AN, Bird MI, Roberts RG, Jacobs Z, et al. Minimum founding populations for the first peopling of Sahul. Nature ecology & evolution. 2019;3(7):1057–1063. pmid:31209287
                              • View Article
                              • PubMed/NCBI
                              • Google Scholar
                            • 57. Eriksson A, Betti L, Friend AD, Lycett SJ, Singarayer JS, von Cramon-Taubadel N, et al. Late Pleistocene climate change and the global expansion of anatomically modern humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2012;109(40):16089–16094. pmid:22988099
                              • View Article
                              • PubMed/NCBI
                              • Google Scholar
                            • 58. Timmermann A, Tobias F. Late Pleistocene climate drivers of early human migration. Nature. 2016;538(7623):92–95. pmid:27654920
                              • View Article
                              • PubMed/NCBI
                              • Google Scholar
                            • 59. Codding BF, Jones TL. Environmental productivity predicts migration, demographic, and linguistic patterns in prehistoric California. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2013;110(36):14569–14573.
                              • View Article
                              • Google Scholar
                            • 60. One Earth Bioregions: https://www.oneearth.org/bioregions-2020/
                              • 61. Dinerstein E, Olson D, Joshi A, Vynne C, Burgess ND, Wikramanayake E, Hahn N, et al. An ecoregion-based approach to protecting half the terrestrial realm. BioScience. 2017;67(6):534–545. pmid:28608869
                                • View Article
                                • PubMed/NCBI
                                • Google Scholar
                              • 62. Bivand RS, Wong DW. Comparing implementations of global and local indicators of spatial association. Test. 2018;27:716–748.
                                • View Article
                                • Google Scholar
                              • 63. Team, R. C. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria; 2020.
                                • 64. Gurven M, Borgerhoff Mulder M, Hooper PL, Kaplan H, Quinlan R, Sear R, et al. Domestication alone does not lead to inequality: intergenerational wealth transmission among horticulturalists. Current Anthropology. 2010;51(1):49–64.
                                  • View Article
                                  • Google Scholar
                                • 65. Shenk MK, Borgerhoff Mulder M, Beise J, Clark G, Irons W, Leonetti D, et al. Intergenerational wealth transmission among agriculturalists: foundations of agrarian inequality. Current Anthropology. 2010;51(1):65–83.
                                  • View Article
                                  • Google Scholar
                                • 66. Smith EA, Hill K, Marlowe FW, Nolin D, Wiessner P, Gurven M, et al. Wealth transmission and inequality among hunter-gatherers. Current Anthropology. 2010;51(1):19–34. pmid:21151711
                                  • View Article
                                  • PubMed/NCBI
                                  • Google Scholar
                                • 67. Chagnon NA. Yanomamo social organization and warfare. In: Fried MH, Harris M, Murphy RF, editors. War: The anthropology of armed conflict and aggression. American Museum of Natural History; 1968. p. 109–159.
                                  • 68. Bird RB, Smith E, Bird DW. The hunting handicap: costly signaling in human foraging strategies. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2001;50(1):9–19.
                                    • View Article
                                    • Google Scholar
                                  • 69. Hawkes K, Bliege Bird R. Showing off, handicap signaling, and the evolution of men’s work. Evolutionary Anthropology. 2002;11(2):58–67.
                                    • View Article
                                    • Google Scholar
                                  • 70. Smith EA, Bliege Bird R. Costly signaling and cooperative behavior. In: Life H, Gintis S, Bowles R, Boyd R, Fehr E, editors. Moral sentiments and material interests: The foundations of cooperation in economic life. MIT Press; 2005. p. 115–148.
                                    • 71. Smith EA, Bliege Bird R, Bird DW. The benefits of costly signaling: Meriam turtle hunters. Behavioral Ecology. 2003;14(1):116–126.
                                      • View Article
                                      • Google Scholar
                                    • 72. Walker RS, Bailey DH. Body counts in lowland South American violence. Evolution and Human Behavior. 2013;34(1):29–34.
                                      • View Article
                                      • Google Scholar
                                    • 73. Von Rueden CR, Jaeggi AV. Men’s status and reproductive success in 33 nonindustrial societies: Effects of subsistence, marriage system, and reproductive strategy. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2016;113(39):10824–10829.
                                      • View Article
                                      • Google Scholar
                                    • 74. Borgerhoff-Mulder MB, Bowles S, Hertz T, Bell A, Beise J, Clark G, et al. Intergenerational wealth transmission and the dynamics of inequality in small-scale societies. Science. 2009;326(5953):682–688. pmid:19900925
                                      • View Article
                                      • PubMed/NCBI
                                      • Google Scholar
                                    • 75. Bliege Bird R, Smith EA. Signaling theory, strategic interaction, and symbolic capital. Current Anthropology. 2005;46(2):221–248.
                                      • View Article
                                      • Google Scholar
                                    • 76. Blurton Jones NG. Tolerated theft, suggestions about the ecology and evolution of sharing, hoarding and scrounging. Biology and Social Life. 1987;26(1):31–54.
                                      • View Article
                                      • Google Scholar
                                    • 77. Kuznar L. Risk sensitivity and value among Andean pastoralists: Measures, models, and empirical tests. Current Anthropology. 2001;42(3):432–440.
                                      • View Article
                                      • Google Scholar
                                    • 78. Winterhalder B, Lu F, Tucker B. Risk-sensitive adaptive tactics: models and evidence from subsistence studies in biology and anthropology. Journal of Archaeological Research. 1999;7(4):301–348.
                                      • View Article
                                      • Google Scholar
                                    • 79. Keeley LH. War before civilization. OUP USA; 1996.
                                      • View Article
                                      • Google Scholar
                                    • 80. Micheletti AJ, Ruxton GD, Gardner A. Why war is a man’s game. Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 2018;285(1884):20180975. pmid:30111597
                                      • View Article
                                      • PubMed/NCBI
                                      • Google Scholar
                                    • 81. Harris M. Animal capture and Yanomamo warfare: Retrospect and new evidence. Journal of Anthropological Research. 1984;40(1):183–201.
                                      • View Article
                                      • Google Scholar

                                    Is a subsistence system that relies on herds of domesticated livestock?

                                    Pastoralism is a subsistence system that relies on herds of domesticated livestock.

                                    What are the 5 basic types of subsistence practices and what characteristics tend to correlate with those 5 basic types?

                                    This encompasses the attainment of nutrition, water, and shelter. The five broad categories of subsistence patterns are foraging, horticulture, pastoralism, agriculture, and industrial food production.

                                    What type of subsistence strategy has been used for most of human history?

                                    Foraging. For roughly 90% of history, humans were foragers who used simple technology to gather, fish, and hunt wild food resources. Today only about a quarter million people living in marginal environments, e.g., deserts, the Arctic and topical forests, forage as their primary subsistence strategy.

                                    What is the main reason why people did not return to foraging quizlet?

                                    -They could not return to foraging because climate change, lack of space, extinction, and skill loss. -This made agriculture the only remaining option. In what ways were early agriculturalists worse off than foragers?